Real-Life Intense Fear is Communicated Through Context, Not Facial Expressions
en-GBde-DEes-ESfr-FR

Real-Life Intense Fear is Communicated Through Context, Not Facial Expressions


A new study challenges the longstanding belief that fear is primarily communicated through facial expressions, showing instead that context plays the dominant role in real-life fear recognition. By analyzing real-life fear reactions in videos, researchers found that facial expressions alone fail to reliably signal fear, whereas situational context—such as the environment and body posture—allows for clear and accurate fear perception. These findings have major implications for psychology, neuroscience, and artificial intelligence, suggesting that current emotion recognition models, including AI-based systems, need to incorporate contextual information rather than relying solely on facial cues.

Recognizing fear in others is crucial for survival, but how do we achieve this? A new study, published in PNAS, led by Professor Hillel Aviezer and PhD student Maya Lecker from the department of Psychology at Hebrew University challenges the widely accepted notion that fear is primarily communicated through facial expressions. Instead, the research finds that context, rather than facial reactions, plays a critical role in fear recognition.

For decades, emotion research has centered on the idea that facial expressions provide a clear, universal signal of fear. However, these studies often rely on posed expressions rather than genuine fear responses. Overcoming significant practical challenges, Professor Aviezer and his team conducted an innovative study analyzing real-life reactions captured in videos during intense fear-inducing situations such as height jumps, physical attacks, and exposure to phobia triggers.

The study, which involved 12 preregistered experiments with a total of 4,180 participants, examined how people perceive fear in real-life scenarios. Participants were shown different versions of video clips: faces alone, context without faces, and full videos combining both elements. The researchers used various methods to assess fear perception, including forced-choice labeling, open-ended responses, and valence-arousal ratings.

The results revealed a striking contrast between commonly held assumptions and real-world observations:
  • Facial expressions alone failed to communicate fear reliably.
  • Context without faces, as well as full videos with context, led to clear and robust recognition of fear.
These findings have implications for multiple fields, including psychology, neuroscience, and artificial intelligence. Many current models of emotion recognition—including AI-based emotion detection systems—rely heavily on facial cues. However, this study suggests that these systems may need to incorporate broader contextual understanding to accurately interpret emotions.

“Our research shows that despite the widely held belief that fear is expressed through a distinctive facial expression, real-life fear reactions tell a different story,” said Professor Aviezer. “Facial expressions alone carry minimal diagnostic value for fear recognition, while situational context plays a crucial role.”

This study paves the way for a more nuanced understanding of how humans perceive emotions, urging researchers, clinicians, and technology developers to reconsider existing models of emotion recognition.
The research paper titled “Real-life intense fear is communicated through context, not facial expressions” is now available in PNAS and can be accessed at https://www.pnas.org/doi/10.1073/pnas.2414677122
DOI 10.1073/pnas.241467712
Funding
This work was supported by the Israel Science Foundation
Researchers:
Maya Lecker1, Scott Hallock2, Axel Danielson3, Maximilien Van Aertrickc3, Merel Kindt4, Hillel Aviezer1
Institutions:
1. Department of Psychology, Hebrew University of Jerusalem
2. WMTI Productions, Inc., Encino, CA
3. Plattform Produktion, Gothenburg
4. Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Amsterdam
Regions: Middle East, Israel
Keywords: Society, Psychology, Applied science, Technology

Disclaimer: AlphaGalileo is not responsible for the accuracy of content posted to AlphaGalileo by contributing institutions or for the use of any information through the AlphaGalileo system.

Testimonials

For well over a decade, in my capacity as a researcher, broadcaster, and producer, I have relied heavily on Alphagalileo.
All of my work trips have been planned around stories that I've found on this site.
The under embargo section allows us to plan ahead and the news releases enable us to find key experts.
Going through the tailored daily updates is the best way to start the day. It's such a critical service for me and many of my colleagues.
Koula Bouloukos, Senior manager, Editorial & Production Underknown
We have used AlphaGalileo since its foundation but frankly we need it more than ever now to ensure our research news is heard across Europe, Asia and North America. As one of the UK’s leading research universities we want to continue to work with other outstanding researchers in Europe. AlphaGalileo helps us to continue to bring our research story to them and the rest of the world.
Peter Dunn, Director of Press and Media Relations at the University of Warwick
AlphaGalileo has helped us more than double our reach at SciDev.Net. The service has enabled our journalists around the world to reach the mainstream media with articles about the impact of science on people in low- and middle-income countries, leading to big increases in the number of SciDev.Net articles that have been republished.
Ben Deighton, SciDevNet

We Work Closely With...


  • BBC
  • The Times
  • National Geographic
  • The University of Edinburgh
  • University of Cambridge
  • iesResearch
Copyright 2025 by AlphaGalileo Terms Of Use Privacy Statement