Children Learn Language from Wholes to Parts, Challenging Traditional Views of Language Acquisition
en-GBde-DEes-ESfr-FR

Children Learn Language from Wholes to Parts, Challenging Traditional Views of Language Acquisition


A new study reveals an often-overlooked process in language learning: children frequently begin by grasping whole phrases and only later identify the individual components within them. This “whole-to-part” approach suggests that children have an inherent bias toward holistic learning, challenging the traditional view that language is usually acquired by assembling smaller parts into larger structures. The findings shed light on the flexibility and resilience of language acquisition across diverse contexts, including situations where formal language input is limited.

A recent study led by Prof. Inbal Arnon from the Department of Psychology at the Hebrew University of Jerusalem, in collaboration with Prof. Susan Goldin-Meadow from the Department of Psychology at the University of Chicago, has unveiled new insight on how children learn language. Traditionally, it has been thought that children approach language by building larger structures, such as words and sentences, out of smaller units, like syllables. Arnon’s past work however, highlights an equally vital process: children sometimes start with whole phrases and later identify the parts within them. In this paper, the researchers show this process in children not exposed to linguistic input: this challenges the idea that children’s approach to language is strictly shaped by the segmented or unsegmented nature of linguistic input. Instead, it suggests that children may bring a natural bias toward holistic learning, helping them to acquire language in versatile ways.

The researchers explored this process by focusing on homesigners—deaf children who, lacking exposure to spoken or sign language, create their own gestural communication systems. These homesign systems share many features of natural languages but are developed independently of a conventional language model. The study revealed that homesigners initially use gestures to represent entire meanings, with no breakdown into distinct components like specific hand shapes or motions. As they grow, however, these children begin to dissect these whole gestures, creating smaller, meaningful parts that they can recombine to convey nuanced meanings. Remarkably, this whole-to-part learning emerged even without linguistic input that typically encourages children to analyze language structure.

“Our findings indicate that children are not only responding to their environment but may come with an inherent preference for certain ways of learning language, including the ability to start with broad meanings and discover parts later,” Prof. Arnon explains. “This has important implications for understanding the flexibility and resilience of language learning across different contexts. Children’s ability to grasp language by moving from wholes to parts may serve early communicative needs, allowing them to convey complex ideas before developing more intricate structures.”

The study’s findings suggest that whole-to-part learning is not just a fallback for situations where language input is difficult to segment; rather, it is an approach that children naturally bring to communication. This insight has broader implications, from understanding how languages evolve and adapt over generations to examining the developmental stages of emerging sign languages such as Nicaraguan Sign Language, which also exhibit a transition from holistic forms to more segmented, combinable parts.

Prof. Arnon and Prof. Goldin-Meadow’s research offers a fresh perspective on language acquisition, highlighting that children may begin with holistic expressions and gradually develop the ability to analyze and recombine parts. This approach underscores the adaptability of human language across diverse contexts, from spoken languages to homesign systems and beyond.
The research paper titled “Whole-to-part development in language creation” is now available in Trends in Cognitive Sciences and can be accessed at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2024.09.015.

Researchers:
Susan Goldin-Meadow1, Inbal Arnon2

Institutions:
1) Department of Psychology, University of Chicago
2) Department of Psychology, Hebrew University of Jerusalem
Regions: Middle East, Israel
Keywords: Society, Psychology, Humanities, Education

Disclaimer: AlphaGalileo is not responsible for the accuracy of news releases posted to AlphaGalileo by contributing institutions or for the use of any information through the AlphaGalileo system.

Témoignages

We have used AlphaGalileo since its foundation but frankly we need it more than ever now to ensure our research news is heard across Europe, Asia and North America. As one of the UK’s leading research universities we want to continue to work with other outstanding researchers in Europe. AlphaGalileo helps us to continue to bring our research story to them and the rest of the world.
Peter Dunn, Director of Press and Media Relations at the University of Warwick
AlphaGalileo has helped us more than double our reach at SciDev.Net. The service has enabled our journalists around the world to reach the mainstream media with articles about the impact of science on people in low- and middle-income countries, leading to big increases in the number of SciDev.Net articles that have been republished.
Ben Deighton, SciDevNet
AlphaGalileo is a great source of global research news. I use it regularly.
Robert Lee Hotz, LA Times

Nous travaillons en étroite collaboration avec...


  • BBC
  • The Times
  • National Geographic
  • The University of Edinburgh
  • University of Cambridge
  • iesResearch
Copyright 2024 by DNN Corp Terms Of Use Privacy Statement